Wednesday, 31 October 2012

My Independent Research Project

Just stretching  <my data i've forgotten how to embed videos

Just Stretching: An Independent Research Project



The data, I have chosen for this independent research project is a breaching experiment. Whilst some components of the data are scripted on the side of the experimenters, my focus is on what breaching experiments do best. What they do best is to break the social order, organisations and scripts, thus exposing concepts and theories at a micro-sociological level. Therefore, while some of this data is scripted, it doesn’t matter because my focus is on the naturally occurring data that is produced as an end result of the experiment. The breaching experiment, ‘Just Stretching’, is set in an average supermarket. The participants are stretching in an institutionalised space, interrupting the social order. As a result many interesting reactions are recorded of subjects reacting to the breach. I’ve analysed the exposed phenomena shown by the data through observations and further research.

Sense-making is an innate reaction to any and all social situations, especially valuable to those where a social norm(s) have been broken. Actors take the situation at face-value, ‘as they see them, so they are’ (Hertiage, 1984, p.77), as well as ‘normal patterns’ and ‘usual occurrences’(Hertiage, 1984, p.77),  to apply sense to the situation. Dervin(1983) called it the filling of the ‘cognitive gap’ and it allows individuals to experience relatively new situations without suffering shock and with a fragmented script with which to form actions. At the very least, this process alerts individuals and actors to flee from situations that presented abnormal patterns of behaviour or red flags that read danger.
In the data, two women are presented with same situation, two young men stretching on opposing sides of a fairly large aisle. The first woman decided ignore the young men’s breaching actions treating them as situation that only required ‘side’ involvement (Goffman,1963, p13-22). She most likely understood that their actions were slightly problematic but otherwise harmless, and choose both not to shift her main focus and to use strategic indifference (which will be explained later) to maintain her focus on shopping activities. The second woman, using her sense-making abilities, took the situation at face-value and decided to flee from the interaction. Either faced with a lack of information or red flags, she took the flight response to breach of social order.

Another interesting reaction to the breaching of social norms was to ‘normalise the discrepancy’. Heritage(1984) talks about this as a reaction to a tic-tac-toe experiment in which participants cheated. Subjects either rejected the new game-play or sort to normalise the discrepancy by cheating also. By doing this they sort to legitimise the new game-play by recognising the observed actions as ‘legally possible events’(Heritage, 1984, p.79). This reaction seemed parallel one of the women at the supermarket where the Just Stretching experiment took place. She was interested and wanted to engage in the observed behaviour that was clearly in the breach of the social organisation and uses of the supermarket. By feeling motivation to attempt to participate in the disruptive behaviour, she was ‘normalising the discrepancy’. Even if this meant also attempting to change the social order of a fairly institutionalised setting on however a small scale.
In Goffman’s seminar piece, ‘Behavior in Public Spaces’ in 1963, he talks about ‘Unfocused Interactions’. Unfocused Interactions are those interactions that not the subjects of a singular focus but rather of spilt focuses as either the ‘main’ or ‘side’ interaction. The idea is that individual in having the brain space to multi-task, can divide focus in interactions, for example a student can listen to a teacher and make doodles on their notepads at the same time, unequal attention is given to both but nevertheless it is spilt. Goffman(1963) asserts that unfocused interactions pre-dominate urban spaces where strangers negotiate interactions. Strangers may never speak but “silent bargains” and “implicit negotiations”(Strauss, 1978, p.224) are constantly occurring and it is here where strategic indifference comes into play.

Strategic Indifference is the observable ability of an individual to use indifference in unfocused interactions to avoid others, who have capacity to impede and interrupt current proceedings. It could be seen as uncivil inattention to Goffman’s (1984) ‘civil inattention’ in that a step is skipped. Instead of first recognising others by giving them person-treatment i.e. looking them in the face, gesturing ‘hello’ or smiling, instead the ‘other’ is avoided, ignored and not recognised as a person. However in modern-times, its different than that, the individual using strategic indifference is pretending not that the ‘other’ is not a person but that they don’t appear on their radar cause they’re too busy or completely oblivious to what is going on around them. It can sometimes be believable though what with individuals pre-occupation with stress and woe these days. In any case, it may seem rude but it serves a purpose. The first woman from the data who used strategic indifference when faced with the two participants stretching in the aisle, had good reason to use this reaction. Understanding, that while she wasn’t engaging the participants with any large amount of focus that they didn’t pose a threat, decided to continue that disengagement as it was strategically useful to continuing her activities. The participates then read her ‘body idioms’(Goffman, 1963) that she’s purposefully ‘giving off’, and implicitly agree not to further impede her activities.
Sense-making and strategic indifference are both reactions observed in persons in the supermarket where the data takes place. Whilst sense-making seems innate and necessary to survival, the strategic indifference seems performed. Goffman argues that ‘body idioms’ can be purposefully or inadvertently given off and this could be equally true with reactions. Whilst some are the product of subconscious activity, others are performed as a result of an individual’s cognitive processes that have deduced a course of action that is useful to dealing with the current interaction. Reactions are mostly perceived as back-stage behaviour is individual caught off guard and thus produces a real response to the interaction. However, from the time we are born, we observe others behaviour and become skilled in the art of ‘impressions management’ (Manning, 2008, p.123). Subsequently, it would make sense for individuals to reproduce behavioural responses that ended in a favourable result such as witnessing others use strategic indifference. Individuals may not have all the information about the situation but they are not inexperienced. In this way, interactions could be a constant battle or power struggle to see which person picks the best actions and reactions.

Power struggles at first seems very evident in my data. Since Foucault states power as ‘exercised,’ and existing ‘only in action (Foucault,1980, p89), it would seem that data that exposes actions and/or reactions would be perfect to view power at a micro level. If a respondent uses strategic indifference as a response to a breach, it would seem like they are retaining power and not giving it away. If a respondent tries to make sense of a breach, they are then using observed, previous knowledge to empower themselves in the interaction.

However from Mayes (2010), we see that power shouldn’t be viewed as a phenomenon that can be taken and given off easily. Rather power is as Foucault suggests is
‘an unstable, fragmented phenomenon based on institutional relations involving different levels of knowledge and expertise, and created and sustained through micro-level interactions’(Gore 1993)

Since power is as well ‘exercised’ then it does involve agency. Agency of which is subject to the knowledge base that the individual has and what knowledge they are able to observe in the interaction.  So when we look at the reactions in the data, we see people exercising power that is controlled by agency and that cannot be taken by others. Reactions are people exercising power of which they can choose to empower themselves or not, through agency.

In talking about power at a micro-level, I came across Strauss’s Negotiated Order. This approach takes all social order as negotiated order and theorises that big to small scale negotiations effect social order and change. Social organisation is contingent upon negotiation to accomplish any time of working system even with social front or script. Therefore the ability to complete any task that requires face to face interaction, even at the smallest scale, relies on negotiation. The reason this ties in with power is that interaction involves two or more people, and while we may have set reactions, just like the first woman who used strategic indifference in the data when face with two young men stretching in the aisle, they rely on a negotiation with the other party. Strategic indifference only works to allow a person to avoid another and thus continue with their activities, if the other person agrees to the limited interaction or doesn’t notice them either i.e. if those stretching in the aisle were to greet the shopper and then engage them in conversation – then the reaction fails to serve its intended purpose. In that way, on however a small scale, the shopper has exercised agency and power and has failed. These almost invisible negotiations that Strauss(1978) calls “silent bargains” and “implicit negotiations”(Maeder & Nadai, 2008, p.4). Reactions like strategic indifference rely on a silent bargain, ‘you leave me alone and I’ll leave you alone’. Negotiations are fundamentally a struggle for power. Thus Strauss’s Negotiated Order theory, illustrates that everyday interactions at a micro level entails a struggle for power.

Another part of the interactions that was interesting was the use of humour on the side of the students hosting the experiment. While they did not detail to others the reason for actively stretching in the supermarket, they did laugh quite a bit when asked. One reason for this could have been that they found the new situation humorous in its capacity to disrupt actions of others, even at a very small scale. Another reason might have been a way to communicate they understood that they were breaching the social order of the interaction. By laughing, it signalled that they are of sound mind and this is not behaviour that they are typically comfortable with. Futhermore, Kuipers (2006) states that laughter not only breaks down barriers with strangers and unite people of different hierarchical positions. Therefore, laughter could have been used by the students to decrease the likelihood of ridicule from strangers for their breaching behaviour.

In conclusion, my data presented a way to observe many different and interesting concepts from the field of micro-sociology. Reactions from individuals at the supermarket presented both innate and performed strategies to dealing with everyday interactions. Sense-making devices used by individuals allows us to face new situations using prior knowledge, observations and experience with without we couldn’t face simple tasks such as shopping at the supermarket. Using these devices, individuals can enact the best tactical reaction, such as normalising the discrepancy and strategic indifference, to deal with unfocused or focused interactions from the strange to the everyday. Power is exercised through action, and it observable in my data. However, it can be hard to discern how it works because of its nature as phenomenon. What we do know is that power is governed by agency and therefore suggests that is cannot be taken or given away easily unless the individual decides to. Although, Strauss’s negotiated order indicates a struggle for power from micro to macro interactions. Finally, humour is observable in the data and has many uses just like swearing does and in this case was a way for students to motivate others to laugh with them. Thus, communicating without words to others their sanity and willingness to conform to everyone else.

References
Dervin, B. (1983). An overview of sense-making research: Concepts, methods and results. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the International Communication Association. Dallas, TX.
Foucault, M 1980, Power/Knowedge, Harvester Press: Brighton
Goffman, E 1963, ‘Behavior in Public Places: Notes on the Social Organization of Gatherings’, New York: The Free Press.
Goffman,E 1971, ‘Performances’, The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life, Harmondsworth, Penguin,, pp28-82
Goffman, E. (1984) 'The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life, Harmondsworth: Penguin
 Gore, J 1993, ‘Struggle for Pedagogies: Critical and Feminist Discourses as Regimes of Truth’, New York: Routledge  

Heritage, John. 1984. “The Morality of Cognition.” Pp. 75-102 in Garfinkel and Ethnomethodology. Cambridge: Polity Press.

Kuipers, G 2006, Good Humor, Bad Taste: A Sociology of the Joke, New York, NY: Mouton de Gruyter.

Maedar, C & Nadai, E 2008, ‘Negotiations at all Points? Interaction and Organisation’, Forum: Qualitative Social Research, Vol. 9, No. 1, Art. 32, http://www.qualitative-research.net/index.php/fqs/article/view/337/735

Mayes, Patricia. 2010. “The discursive construction of identity and power in the critical classroom: Implications for applied critical theories.” Discourse and Society vol. 21, no. 2: pp. 189-210.

Strauss, A 1978, ‘Negotiations: varieties, contexts, processes, and social order’, Jossey-Bass publishers, London.




No comments:

Post a Comment